Journal Screenshot

International Journal of Academic Research in Business and Social Sciences

Open Access Journal

ISSN: 2222-6990

Preschool Children’s Level of Semantic Acquisition Through Children’s Songs, Stories and Animated Films on Youtube

Kamariah Kamarudin, Zaitul Azma Zainon Hamzah, Pabiyah Haji Maming, Ida Baizura Bahar, Haliza Abdul Rahman, Nor Shahila Mansor

http://dx.doi.org/10.6007/IJARBSS/v13-i17/19820

Open access

Semantic acquisition is the process of a child acquiring eclectic meanings of linguistic elements and conceptual senses. This gradual process starts before a child says their first word, which includes many different types of words. The use of digital technology approaches to examine children’s linguistic environment can benefit children’s language development and make more detailed predictions about what, when, where and how semantic development assists children’s language development. Hence, this study attempts to identify the level of semantic acquisition of preschool children using children’s songs and stories as well as animated films in YouTube and summarise the types of words children acquire from them. This study employed the observation method and its study sample comprised 50 preschool children selected using a purposive sampling technique. The research data consisted of two children’s stories, three children’s songs and two animated films downloaded from YouTube. The findings show that the acquisition of synonyms happens at a basic level, 94%. Similarly, the acquisition of antonyms occurs at a basic level, 38%, while the acquisition of hyponyms occurs at a low level, 44%. Therefore, it established that the level of acquisition for hyponyms is better than the levels of acquisition for synonyms and antonyms. The data of this study ascertained that the developments of semantics, that is, the mastery of synonyms, antonyms and hyponyms among children, occur at different levels. Additionally, this study’s results recorded uneven children’s lexical or vocabulary developments. Hence, creating a linguistic atmosphere that is more fitting for preschool children is imperative because one can use it to explore the meanings of words and concepts among them in order to further enhance their semantic acquisitions, in addition to using children’s songs and stories on YouTube.

Barrett, M. (1995). Early lexical development. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 362–392). Blackwell.
Brown, C.S. (2014). Language and literacy development in the early years: Foundational skills that support emergent readers. Language and Literacy Spectrum, 24, 35-49.
Clark, E. V. (1973). What’s in a word? On the child’s acquisition of semantics in his first language. Cognitive Development and Acquisition of Language. 65-110. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-505850-6.50009-8
Clark, E. V. (2015). Semantics and language acquisition in the handbook of contemporary semantic theory. In S. Lappin & C. Fox (Eds.), The handbook of contemporary semantic theory (pp. 714-733). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118882139.ch23
Creswell (2012). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among
five approaches. 3rd ed. SAGE Publications Ltd.
De Deyne, S., Navarro, A. P., Brysbaert, M., & Storms, G. (2019). The “small world of words” English word association norms for over 12,000 cue words. Behavior Research Methods 51, 987-1006. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1115-7
De Deyne, S., Navarro, D. J., & Storms, G. (2013). Better explanations of lexical and semantic cognition using networks derived from continued rather than single word associations. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 480–498. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0260-7
Dick, F., Tierney, A., Lutti, A., Josephs, O., Sereno, M. I., & Weiskopf, N. (2012). In vivo functional and myeloarchitectonic mapping of human primary auditory areas. J. Neurosci. 32, 16095–16105. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1712-12.2012
Dubossarsky, H., De Deyne, S., & Hills, T. T. (2017). Quantifying the structure of free association networks across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 53(8), 1560. DOI: 10.1037/dev0000347
Tribushinina, E., van den Bergh, H., Ravid, D., Aksu-Koc, A., Kilani-Scoch, M., Korecky-Kroll, K., Leibovitch-Cohen, I., Laaha, S., Nir, B, Dressler, W., & Gillis, S. (2014) Development of adjective frequencies across semantic classes: A growth curve analysis of child speech and child-directed speech. Language, Interaction and Acquisition, 5(2)185–226. DOI: 10.1075/lia.5.2.02tri
Ferguson. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. American Anthropologist. 66(6), 103-114. http://www.jstor.org/stable/668164
Dick, F., Krishnan, S., Leech, R., & Curtin, S. (2016). Language development. In G. Hickok & S. L. Small (Eds.), Neurobiology of language (pp. 373-388). Elsevier. DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-407794-2.00031-6
Greenwood, C. R., Thiemann-Bourque, K., Walker, D., Buzhardt, J., & Gilkerson, J. (2011). Assessing children’s home language environments using automatic speech recognition technology. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 32(2), 83–92.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740110367826
Haviland, S. E., & Clark, E. V. (1974). ‘This man's father is my father's son’: A study of the acquisition of English kin terms. Journal of Child Language, 1(1), 23 – 47. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900000064
Hayes, D. P., & Ahrens, M. G. (1988). Vocabulary simplification for children: A special case of “motherese?” Journal of Child Language, 15(2), 395–410.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900012411
Hills, T. T. (2013). The company that words keep: Comparing the statistical structure of child- versus adult-directed language. Journal of Child Language, 40(3), 586–604. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000912000165
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343-365. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.08.002
Huang, I., & Lee, H. W. (1945). Experimental analysis of child animism. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 66(1), 69-74. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1945.10533311
Pacesova, J. (1987). Semantic development: theory and application. Studia Minora Facultati8 Philosophicae Univebsitati Bbunensis.
Jimenez, E., & Hills, T. T. (2017, 26-29 July). Network analysis of a large sample of typical and late talkers [Paper presentation]. 2017 The 39th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, London, United Kingdom.
Winters, K., & Griffin S. M. (2014). Singing is a celebration of language: Using music to enhance young children’s vocabularies. Language and Literacy, 16(3), 78-91. DOI:10.20360/G2ZK5X
Kamus Dewan Edisi Keempat. (2010). Dewan Bahasa dan Pustaka.
Kawulich, B. B. (2015). Collecting data through observation. In C. Wagner, B. Kawulich & M. Garner (Eds.), Doing social research: A global context (pp.150-160). McGraw Hill.
Krashen, S. D., & Terrell, T. D. (1983). The natural approach: Language acquisition in the
classroom. Prentice Hall Elt.
Wagner, L. (2010). The acquisition of semantics. Cognitive Science, 1, 519-526. DOI:10.1002/wcs.24
Lee, H. (2021). Children and egocentrism. https://educationgateshead.org/wp-content/uploads/2021/09/3535b-JH-Children-And-Egocentrism.pdf
Parnes, M., & Pagano, M. (2022). Infant and child development: From conception through late childhood. Pressbooks.
Marshall, C., & Rossman, G. B. (1989). Designing qualitative research. SAGE Publications.
McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2012). Self-regulation in early childhood: Improving conceptual clarity and developing ecologically valid measures. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 136–142. DOI:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00191.x
Dosenbach, N. U. F., Nardos, B., Cohen, A. L., Fair, D. A., Power, J. D., Church, J. A., Nelson, S. M., Wig, G. S., Vogel, A. C., Lessov-Schlaggar, C. N., Barnes, K. A., Dubis, J.W., Feczko, E., Coalson, R. S., Pruett, Jr. J. R., Barch, D. M., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2010). Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science, 329(5997), 1358-1361. doi: 10.1126/science.1194144
Džani?, N. D., & Peji?, A. (2016). The effect of using songs on young learners and their motivation for learning English. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 1 (2), 40-54. DOI:10.24819/netsol2016.8
Ninio, A. (1988). On formal grammatical categories in early child language. In Y. Levy, I.M.
Schlesinger & M. D. S. Braine (Eds.), Categories and processes in language acquisition (pp. 11–30). Erlbaum.
Oller, D. K, Niyogi, P., Gray, S., Richards, J. A., Gilkerson, J., Xu, D., Yapanel, U., & Warren, S. F. (2010). Automated vocal analysis of naturalistic recordings from children with autism, language delay, and typical development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(30), 13354-13359. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1003882107
Piaget, J. (1923). The language and thought of the child (1st ed.). Routledge.
Rabiah, S. (2018). Language as a tool for communication and cultural reality discloser. INA-Rxiv. https://doi.org/10.31227/osf.io/nw94m
Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). Semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach. MIT Press.
Saffran, J. R. (2014). Sounds and meanings working together: Word learning as a collaborative effort. Language Learning, 64(2), 106-120. doi: 10.1111/lang.12057
Salerni, N., Assanelli, A., D’Odorico, L., & Rossi, G. (2007). Qualitative aspects of productive vocabulary at the 200- and 500-word stages: A comparison between spontaneous speech and parental report data. First Language, 27(1), 75–87. https://doi.org/10.1177/014272370706754
Sereno, M. I., Lutti, A., Weiskopf, N., & Dick, F. (2013) Mapping the human cortical surface by combining quantitative T(1) with retinotopy. Cerebral Cortex, 23(9), 2261-2268. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhs213
Silliman, E. R., & Wilkinson, L. C. (2004). Language and literacy learning in schools. Guilford Publications.
Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2003). Development as a dynamic system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(8), 343-348. https://doi.org/10. 1016/S1364-6613(03)00156-6
Smith, L. B., Jayaraman, S., Clerkin, E., & Yu, C. (2018). The developing infant
creates a curriculum for statistical learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
22(4), 325–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. tics.2018.02.004
Soderstrom, M. (2007). Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Developmental Review, 27(4), 501-532. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.06.002
Stella, M., Beckage, N., & Brede, M. (2017). Multiplex lexical networks reveal patterns in early word acquisition in children. Scientific Reports ,7(46730), 1-10.
https://doi.org/10.1038/srep46730
Teemant, A., & Pinnegar, S. E. (2019). Proficiency levels defined. In B. Allman (Ed.), Principles of language acquisition.
https://edtechbooks.org/language_acquisition/proficiency_levels_defined
Thomas, R. M. (1979). Comparing theories of child development. Wadsworth.
Wangru, C. (2016). Vocabulary teaching based on semantic-field. Journal of Education and Learning, 5(3). doi:10.5539/jel.v5n3p64
Wiestler, T., & Diedrichsen, J. (2013). Skill learning strengthens cortical representations of motor sequences. Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London.
Wojcik, E. H., & Saffran, J. R. (2013). The ontogeny of lexical networks toddlers encodes the relationships among referents when learning novel words. Psychological Science, 24(10), 1898–1905. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613478198

N/A